Users of psychedelic substances, from traditional shamans to modern-day therapy patients and recreational users, have often reported ESP experiences occurring in the drug-state. Parapsychologists, keen to discover whether this altered state of consciousness might prove a reliable way to induce clairvoyance and telepathy, carried out a number surveys and experimental studies before the 1960s, when such research was brought to an end by the virtual global ban on psychedelics. However, the restrictions on such research are gradually easing, opening a prospect of the relationship between psi and psychedelics being better understood.
- Definition
- Field Reports of Intentional and Spontaneous Psi Phenomena
- Surveys of Paranormal Belief and Experience in Relation to Drug Use
- Experimental Psi Research with Psychedelics
- Methodological Critique of the Experimental Research and Suggestions for Future Study
- Overview of Psychedelic Psi Research
- Literature
- Endnotes
Definition
According to Grinspoon and Bakalar, a psychedelic substance is ‘one which, without causing physical addiction, craving, major physiological disturbances, delirium, disorientation, or amnesia, more or less reliably produces thought, mood, and perceptual changes otherwise rarely experienced except in dreams, contemplative and religious exaltation, flashes of vivid involuntary memory, and acute psychoses.’1
Field Reports of Intentional and Spontaneous Psi Phenomena
Archaeological evidence suggests psychoactive plants have been ingested the world over for millennia,2 and there are many examples in the literature of anthropology and ethnobotany of this leading to the appearance of ostensible psi phenomena3 – despite reservations on the part of anthropologists against reporting them.4 Reports of paranormal experiences in this context became more frequent following the discovery of psychedelic compounds by the academic community, and their popularization by the novelist Aldous Huxley5 and others. This attracted the attention of parapsychologists, who embarked on research of psi in relation to psychedelics, primarily from the 1950s and 1960s6. JB Rhine, the founder of parapsychology as an experimental science, ran informal psychedelic sessions in 1961 in collaboration with the then Harvard psychologists Timothy Leary and Richard Alpert,7 although the sessions apparently generated too much spontaneous laughter for anyone to credibly test for anything.8
Several other parapsychologists have reported personal ESP experiences with the use of psychedelics.9 Parapsychologist Stanley Krippner’s apparent precognitive vision of President Kennedy’s assassination while on psilocybin a year before serves as an example.10 Similar reports of ESP being experienced by consciousness researchers, chemists, anthropologists and psychonauts can also be found elsewhere in the literature.11
There are surprisingly few published psychiatric inpatient reports relating to psychedelics and paranormal experience.12 This may be due to the lack of spontaneous phenomena within the psychiatric population, or to the medicalization within psychiatry of paranormal experiences as ‘delusions’ or ‘hallucinations’. Indeed, Mogar noted that early psychoanalytic and behaviourist researchers using LSD were prejudiced against ESP phenomena.13 Yet one study, a psychiatric-interview survey with users of LSD, 14 reported precognitive experiences as one of the symptoms of the LSD flashback phenomena, now referred to as ‘hallucinogen persisting perception disorder’.15
On the other hand, many accounts of paranormal experiences with psychoactive drugs can be found in the field of psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy, from therapists who write about their work in detail, and these tend to occur with greater frequency than in non-psychedelic therapy16. The psychiatrist Stanislav Grof – who is generally credited with the greatest expertise in this field, having conducted more than 4,000 psychedelic therapy sessions over a two-decade period – claimed to have observed patients experiencing ESP – particularly precognition and accurate remote viewing – on a daily basis. 17 The occurrence of extraordinary coincidences, or synchronicities, was the most frequent type of experience but, curiously, these occurred only among those clients who experienced transpersonal breakthroughs within the psychedelic session.18
A wealth of reports attests to the spontaneous occurrence of ostensible psi phenomena with the use of psychedelic substances, but such experiences are not necessarily genuine psi events. Aside from the usual arguments for and against spontaneous phenomena as evidence for the paranormal,19 the fact that respondents had consumed a vision-inducing substance may be considered reason to question the accuracy of their perception and the interpretation of their experiences – at least for cases that are not substantiated by evidence or independent observers. Nevertheless, Shanon20 pointed out that the usual definitions of hallucination in the psychological literature fail to adequately encompass the diverse and complex nature of experiences that occur with psychedelics, nor can assumptions be made about the ontology (the reality) of such psychedelic-induced visions. Indeed, it is common for people who have had ostensibly paranormal psychedelic experiences to emphasise how real they felt, more real even than ordinary waking experience.21 Empirical research is needed to validate these claims.
Surveys of Paranormal Belief and Experience in Relation to Drug Use
Two trends can quickly be perceived in the research. First, virtually all of the sixteen surveys reviewed were published since the 1970s, following the ban on the use of psychoactive drugs that led to an end to research with humans (although this research is now slowly resuming). Second, most of the surveys primarily focused on paranormal experiences (nine surveys) and/or belief (seven surveys); they also tended to record drug use information as one of many possible co-variables (ten surveys), often omitting to distinguish among the different substances. Only six studies specifically approached users of psychoactive substances as the target sample.22
The findings from the survey research indicate a small but consistent – and typically significant – relationship (r = .16 to .25) between paranormal belief and drug use, although its size was more pronounced among marijuana users in one study.23 Furthermore, these studies support the hypothesis that psychedelics can induce some paranormal experiences, although the arguments that are advanced for and against the genuineness of field reports also prevail here.24 Of the reviewed surveys, correlations between the occurrence of paranormal experiences (including psi and mystical experiences) and the use of all drugs (excluding prescription drugs) ranged from r = .13 to .46, and were typically significant. In addition, or alternatively, those reporting ESP and anomalous or paranormal experiences were found to be significantly more likely to use psychedelics. One study recorded the same finding for recurrent spontaneous psychokinesis (also known as poltergeist phenomena), although only tentatively.25
Of those reporting the use of psychedelics, between 18% and 83% reported ESP experiences – most commonly telepathy but also precognition – actually occurring during drug use, with heavier users reporting more experiences where specified. Yet, conversely, the occurrence of psychokinesis during drug use was only reported to occur by between 13% and 22% of those using psychedelics.26
Where specified, the relationship with paranormal experiences, belief in the paranormal, transliminality, and tolerance of ambiguity is much reduced with cocaine, heroin, and alcohol, compared to psychedelics, perhaps reflecting Metzner’s classification of these former substances as consciousness-contracting drugs.27 The same is also true for the negative relationship with the fear of psi and use of psychedelics, which is not apparent with heroin and actually reversed with alcohol, although replication and analyses for specific classes of drugs is needed to confirm this.28
Correlations between self-reports of cannabis use and ‘thought transmission’ in psychiatric research are apparent, often indirectly, though telepathy experiences are also more widely reported in the apparent absence of pathology, with or without cannabis. Most of the surveys failed to adequately identify which substances lead to which experiences, although a switch to such taxonomic research is now evident. 29 Substances particularly favourable to the experience of telepathy were found to be cannabis, MDMA and DXM. No one substance was particularly generative of precognitive experiences, and possible candidates for clairvoyance were cannabis, LSD, and psilocybin. As yet no research has explored both state and trait personality dimensions in relation to substance-induced psi experiences, although such research is encouraged.30
Experimental Psi Research with Psychedelics
The earliest parapsychology experiments with psychoactive substances, published between 1943 and 1961, were conducted with simple stimulants and depressants such as caffeine, amphetamine, alcohol, amytal, and quinal-barbitone, with mixed results.31
The following table summarises all controlled experiments into ESP with psychedelics (‘ns’ = not significant, that is, findings showed no statistical significance; ‘sig +ve’ = significantly positive, that is, statistically-significant positive results were obtained; under ‘Type’, ESP = general ESP card guessing, or not clearly specified, P = psychometry, T = telepathy, C = clairvoyance.)
Study |
Substance |
N |
Type |
Control condition |
Results |
Forced Choice |
|||||
Rush & Cahn (1958) |
mescaline |
3 |
ESP |
? |
ns |
Puharich (1959, 1962) |
A. muscaria |
26 |
ESP |
pre-drug |
sig +ve
|
Whittesley (1960) |
LSD |
27 |
ESP |
pre-drug |
ns |
Langdon-Davies (1961) |
mescaline |
1 |
ESP |
no |
sig +ve |
Masters & Houston (1966) |
LSD |
27* |
ESP |
no |
sig +ve |
Asperen de Boer et al (1966) |
psilocybin |
36 |
ESP |
no-drug group |
sig +ve (ns vs. control) |
Pahnke (1971) |
LSD |
5 |
ESP |
pre-drug |
ns |
Kugel (1977) |
LSD |
? |
T |
pre-drug |
ns |
Tinoco (1994) |
ayahuasca |
1 |
T & C |
no |
ns |
Don et al (1996) |
ayahuasca |
? |
C |
no |
ns |
Free Response |
|||||
Rush & Cahn (1958) |
mescaline |
3 |
P |
? |
+ve (no stats) |
Smythies (1960, 1987) |
mescaline |
1 |
P |
no |
+ve (no stats) |
Osis (1961) |
LSD |
6 |
P |
no |
+ve (no stats) |
Asperen de Boer et al (1966) |
psilocybin |
36 |
P & C |
no-drug group |
sig +ve (ns vs. control) |
Rouhier (1925, 1927) |
mescaline |
6 |
C |
no |
+ve (no stats) |
Cavanna & Servadio (1964) |
LSD or psilocybin |
3 |
C |
pre-drug |
+ve (no stats) |
Wezelman & Bierman (1997) |
cannabis |
36* |
C |
no-drug group |
+ve vs. control (ns vs. MCE) |
Wezelman & Bierman (1997) |
psilocybin |
6* |
C |
no |
sig. +ve (stacking effect) |
Puharich (1962) |
A. muscaria |
4 |
T |
pre-drug |
sig. +ve (optional stop) |
Asperen de Boer et al (1966) |
psilocybin |
36 |
T |
no-drug group |
ns |
Masters & Houston (1966) |
LSD |
62* |
T |
|
+ve (no stats) |
Bierman (1988) |
psilocybin |
20* |
T |
pre-drug |
ns |
Tinoco (1994) |
ayahuasca |
3 |
T |
no |
ns |
* Study reports that participants were experienced with psychedelics
So far there have been only eighteen published papers, comprising 23 separate experiments, into the efficacy of psychedelics for inducing ESP, primarily with LSD or psilocybin, but also with mescaline, cannabis, Amanita muscaria, and ayahuasca (for a summary, see Table 1). The results of these experiments, which began in the 1950s, varied in their degree of success, most likely in relation to the methodology involved.32 The most successful experiments tended to utilize participants who were experienced in the use of psychedelics, and also utilized free-response testing procedures, with open-ended mentation regarding their internal state rather than forced-choice guessing scenarios, which tend to be repetitive and thus rather boring, especially in the drug-induced state. Indeed, Luke suggests that the more naïvely-designed projects lost any hope of sensibly testing for anything, let alone psi, once their inexperienced participants succumbed to the mystical rapture of their first trip.33
These few experiments were also reported quite differently, sometimes as entire monographs in excess of a hundred pages, at other times as footnotes within other published reports, often lacking useful details and statistics. The majority were essentially pilot studies, mostly conducted during the psychedelic research period of the 1960s.
Due to the exploratory nature of most of these experiments, it is difficult to fully assess their efficacy in using psychedelics to produce ESP (no psychokinesis experiments having been attempted). In most cases the study could have largely been improved with an adequate control condition for order effects,34 also the masked use of decoy targets in the judging process. Procedures using subjective probability estimates by experimenters (such as Asperen de Boer, Barkema, & Kappers35) are now virtually obsolete in parapsychology, being difficult to assess and prone to bias.36 Of ten ESP-card experiments, in the one that used a control condition, the scores in the psilocybin condition were significantly different from chance and were also superior to the control condition, although not significantly.37 Nevertheless, experiments using forced choice ESP-card type symbol guessing procedures were largely unsuccessful compared to chance expectation. Indeed, the use of the symbol-guessing procedure has been widely criticized for being far too mundane under the influence of psychedelics.38 Even so, using Amanita muscaria, Puharich39 showed that forced-choice procedures could be successful with picture-sorting tasks, although there are concerns that Puharich’s experiments were not well controlled for possible sensory leakage.
Alternatively, more engaging, free-response procedures have demonstrated at least some success in all but one of the four studies that used psychometry – the supposed ability of psychically determining the provenance of a given object – although rarely with any control condition for comparison. A clearer indication of possible psychedelic-induced ESP, at times in comparison to a control condition, comes from the four clairvoyance and four telepathy designs, which were mostly positive.40 Despite some promising trends, however, replication is needed and, in most cases, with better methodology and pre-planned analyses. It remains curious that no formal experiments with precognition or psychokinesis have been carried out, particularly the former, considering that powers of divination are traditionally attributed to many plant psychedelics.
Methodological Critique of the Experimental Research and Suggestions for Future Study
When consideration is given to what has been learned from these largely pilot studies, experimenters and commentators alike have highlighted the difficulties involved in attempting to test for psi with participants who have taken a psychedelic. Asperen de Boer et al.41 suggested that participants’ willingness to perform in the task was important, but given the difficulty of maintaining alertness, self-control, focus, interest and orientation to the task,42 equal or more importance should be given to the participants’ capability of performing in the experiment, rather than mere willingness.43
Parker44 notes that a participant’s increased sensitivity to subtle influences under psychedelics is both a boon and a bane to research. Luke45 points out that the qualities that make such research alluring also make for poor test participants, as they may become engrossed in one or more aspects of the experience – the aesthetic rapture,46 the quest for philosophical knowledge,47 soul-searching self-examination,48 their personal drama49 – or simply in observing the flow of their thoughts.50 In addition, participants may struggle to find adequate ways to describe51 the overwhelming flood of ideas and emotions,52 and the speed of change of the internal experience.53 The experience of dissociation (for instance with ketamine) can also hinder communication when participants are no longer present or aware of their physical environment, and, as Huxley notes54, there is a need to reassure participants of their very identity once constructs of space and time disappear.
Despite such drawbacks, it is apparent that obstacles to research may be alleviated or even eliminated if participants in research are experienced in the use of psychedelics.55 Indeed, about a quarter of inexperienced participants are expected to have intense spontaneous mystical experiences during their first trip.56 Only three of the nineteen studies57 specifically reported the use of experienced participants, and these were relatively more successful at getting above-chance psi scores than those that used inexperienced participants.58 Further, it has been suggested that experienced participants can be trained more easily to stabilize their experience59 and may even train themselves to achieve this naturally through repeated use.60 Regardless of training, it has been strongly advised that participants be allowed to stabilize their experience before testing begins.61 Pahnke62 further recommended an eight to twelve-hour pre-dose preparation period when working with terminal cancer patients, although Ludwig63 questioned the necessity of this in a parapsychological context.
Stabilization of the experience may even be expedited by inducing hypnosis prior to drug administration64 in what has been called the ‘hypnodelic’ state.65 Alternatively, Ryzl66 reported re-inducing LSD states through hypnosis, as is also reported elsewhere; it was uncertain how successful this was,67 though success inducing other drug states (such as MDMA and heroin) has been reported.68 It may be that the entire range of psychedelic experiences can be hypnotically re-induced in experienced users, so that no psychedelics need actually be taken during the test procedure. Testing for psi under such ‘controlled flashbacks’ may overcome most of the stipulated problems, with the added advantage of investigating D Scott Rogo’s69 question of whether psi should be attributed to the neurochemical action of the drugs or the state induced thereby. Nevertheless, this approach somewhat restricts participants to those who are both experienced and comfortable with psychedelics and are also highly suggestible.
Some researchers70 suggest that scores in drug studies would improve with the use of psychics and mediums. However, the famous medium Eileen Garrett71 noted that, while LSD enhanced her mediumistic experience, it did not improve her forced choice test-scores. Echoing this, Karlis Osis72 found that mediums were no more successful than normal participants in the other psychometry experiments. This may be explained by the problems associated with inexperienced psychedelic users, indicating that sample selection should primarily seek to identify experienced users in preference to psi-effective participants, although presumably, ideal participants would be both. It is also noted that in traditional shamanic cultures mediumship and the use of psychedelics are rarely found together.73
Considering aspects of timing, several authors have offered advice, though no formal studies have been conducted. Both Ryzl74 and Grof75 suggested that the optimum period for testing psi with LSD was towards the end of the session, when the effects were levelling off – as in the Masters and Houston76 experiments. However Pahnke77 disagreed. As regards to the duration of the psi-task, in preference to the extended test periods favoured by some researchers,78 Osis79 suggested twenty minutes as the maximum for optimum performance.
Some consideration has also been given to the optimal substance. Pahnke80 recommended combining stimulants with psychedelics, whereas Asperen de Boer et al.81 preferred psilocybin to LSD, due to it being milder; Cavanna and Servadio82 agreed. Indeed, LSD has a much longer duration of action than psilocybin and as Blewett83 noted, ten-hour trips are hard to staff. Ryzl84 also questioned the utility of LSD in psychedelic psi testing; he proposed that the ideal substance, if it can be synthesized, should inhibit cortical activity to suppress the stream of thoughts, depress sub-cortical activity to block incoming stimuli, and excite spheres of the cortex involved in ESP production, while maintaining rational insight and increasing suggestibility. Such a designer drug is far in the future, however. Meanwhile, previously unfamiliar ethnobotanical substances are becoming known that have traditionally been used for psychic purposes, but which have not yet been thoroughly tested, or even tested at all, for instance Salvia divinorum.85
Tart86 has further suggested that marijuana is an ideal substance for psi experimentation because of its wide familiarity, its mild psychedelic qualities, and its reputed ability to induce psi, experientially at least. Puharich’s87 apparent repeated success with Amanita muscaria also needs replicating. Other, non-psychedelic chemical psi research, such as Pablos’s88 unsuccessful first-person precognitive-dream drug-study, could also be replicated with the use of psychedelic substances that have been reported to induce psi in dreams, such as the ostensibly precognitive dreams described by both traditional users and by modern consciousness researchers in relation to substances such as Calea zacatechichi,89 Silene capensis,90 and tree datura (Brugmansia).91
Telepathy-like experiments might also benefit from the empathogenic effect of substances such as MDMA: the single participant who was placed under its influence in a remote detection experiment did exceedingly well92, and reports of telepathy are typical with this substance.93 Group telepathy with people under the influence of DXM has also been independently reported by numerous survey respondents.94 The use of a placebo in a double-masked or masked control condition, as in Cavanna and Servadio,95 is of questionable utility in this type of experiment, because at anything less than sub-threshold doses the participant is likely to easily detect the effects of the drug; nevertheless, researchers should be aware that placebo drug effects have been demonstrated in ESP research, when coupled with positive false feedback on task performance.96 One way in which researchers have attempted to circumnavigate the poor disguise of psychedelics, and reduce expectancy effects, in non-parapsychological research. is to tell the participant in advance that they may receive a placebo or one of a number of different drugs, only one of which is a psychedelic, though ultimately participants can usually tell when they get a psychedelic dose.
Several researchers have also commented on the importance of dosage.97 Indeed, Blewett98 warned that giving participants low doses of LSD may not be sufficient to break through the barrier between the normal and the full-blown psychedelic state, and as a result be merely disorientating rather than transformative. Support for this logic is also evident in escalating dose research with DMT.99 It is additionally advised that experienced participants control their own dosage,100 as in the experiment by Wezelman and Bierman.101 Participants’ self-reports of the depth of the altered state were considered better than dosages as indicators of subjective effects.102 Self-reports using the Hallucinogenic Rating Scale were seen to be more accurate indicators of dosage than physiological measures,103 though the use of a scale of transpersonal experience, such as the Self-Expansiveness Scale Form,104 would also likely be fruitful in discerning the relevant depth of the psychedelic state of consciousness. Furthermore, some researchers,105 have noted that the issue of dosage is largely irrelevant in comparison to the influence of the psychological factors of set and setting, as originally noted in psychedelic research by Leary, Litwin, and Metzner.106 In discussion of this, Vayne107 suggested that the influence of psychological factors on psychoactive drugs can vary their effects so much that the drug can be thought of primarily as an experience, composed of set, setting, and substance.
Factors considered important in determining psychological set include the participants’ expectations, their attitudes towards themselves, their idiosyncratic perceptions, and their emotional orientation to the experiment.108 It is also deemed imperative to engender a sense of self-surrender, acceptance, and trust.109 Factors considered important in determining psychological setting include those that are ordinarily considered under demand characteristics,110 particularly the experimenter’s attitude, which should be warm, friendly, and supportive.111 Psychological issues induced through interpersonal relations within the laboratory become magnified when participants are on psychedelics.112 Indeed, Cavanna and Servadio113 highlighted this when one of their participants had an anxiety attack concurrent with their own anxiety, which led them to advise that experimenters themselves should be experienced users of the substance under investigation, as echoed by Strassman.114
Tart115 also recommended that the experimenter should guide the experience towards the goal of the study, and criticized previous work that assumed psychedelic states will automatically induce psi: as noted by Tart, Osmond, and Beloff,116 in traditional scenarios, the shamans who use these substances usually have extensive training and experience. It is further suggested that the experimental task be shaped to the state of the participant, not vice versa,117 and utilize the strong motivation, directed awareness, and complex ritual that is found in shamanism.118 Grob and Harman119 have also urged the integration of aspects from shamanic practices into scientific procedure, with attention being directed to factors of set and setting such as intention, expectation, preparation, group identification, and formalized structure, as well as the integration of the experience in the following months. Indeed, a multi-method approach to studying psychedelic shamanic practices is advised, so that ethnography can inform suitable experimentation.120
Nevertheless, Storm and Rock121 pointed out that, in psi research with psychedelics, researchers need to be aware of the difference between shamanic techniques and merely shamanic-like techniques; for example, the latter may lack the purpose of serving one’s community. Tart122 recommended the implementation of mutual research, where participants are considered as co-investigators, as a means to reduce experimenter bias and enhance a sense of participation, trust, and motivation. One way to ensure such congenial factors in the experimental setting may be to have an experienced psychedelic user and parapsychologist as both the experimenter and participant,123 concerns over placebo effects notwithstanding.
Overview of Psychedelic Psi Research
Even though the subjective paranormal experiences, clinical observations and anthropological reports are subject to all the usual criticisms and rebuttals that apply to non-experimental cases124 there is a growing body of reports, rooted in thousands of years of traditional psychedelic use, that supports the notion that genuine psi phenomena do occur in psychedelic states. As evidence these data are not scientifically rigorous, but they have great value in mapping the phenomenological terrain of psi experiences with psychedelics. This body of reports is further supported by correlations from surveys linking psychedelic use with the increased reporting of psi experiences and belief in psi and the paranormal, although self-reports have more phenomenological merit than evidential value. Furthermore, even though it can be considered little more than exploratory at this stage, the experimental evidence is mostly positive and proves promising so far, illuminating both methodological pitfalls and possibilities.
It is apparent that parapsychopharmacology is a multidisciplinary endeavour, pooling expertise from anthropology, ethnobotany, phytochemistry, neurobiology, psychopharmacology, psychiatry, psychotherapy, transpersonal psychology, and indeed parapsychology. It also owes much to the non-academic explorers of consciousness, be they shamans, occultists, or psychonauts. This branch of research is still very much in its infancy, and, along with other fields conducting research with the use of psychedelics, has been operating very quietly since the late sixties, until a gentle turn in the tide during the last twenty years or so has seen experimental research resumed.125 Nevertheless, experimental research continues to be constrained by requirements for strict ethical and often governmental approval before it can proceed, requiring lengthy applications.126
Tart127 recommended bypassing these difficulties by casually enrolling participants who were already using psychedelics, rather than having the experimenter administer the substances directly. An example of this kind of experiment involved several thousand Grateful Dead fans, renowned for their psychedelic consumption, who acted as senders in a series of dream telepathy experiments, with some success.128 Indeed, taking what Giesler129 calls a psi-in-process approach, and keeping naturalistic variables intact, group experiments may be one way to access the kind of group telepathy experiences that people tripping in groups sometimes report,130 especially on DXM.131 However, without the grounded and controlled atmosphere of a concert or shamanic ceremony, psychedelic group ESP experiments run the risk of turning into bacchanalian scenes, as reported in Puharich’s experiments by his wife.132
Ideally, direct parapsychological research with psychedelics would expand beyond those countries that have legal access to such substances, such as the Netherlands and Brazil – the latter being the only place where experimental psychedelic parapsychology research has been conducted since the 1970s. Furthermore, treating these substances like any other drug worthy of investigation within a medical or therapeutic context has recently proven a fruitful means of inquiry for many researchers,133 although psi research does not readily attract such funding at the present time. Nevertheless, it should be noted that psychedelics are considered as sacramentals by the spiritual and religious groups that use them and they must be utilized and researched with respect.
Luke134 suggests that, besides trying to replicate promising free-response studies, experimental psychedelic research in the future should utilize protocols that maximize psi effects. This work can then simultaneously enhance process research methodology, by indicating optimal conditions for psi through the psychologically magnifying effects of these substances. For instance, Bierman’s135 psychedelic psi research may have revealed the apparent psychic blocking of negative images, and from earlier experiments, that forced-choice tasks are clearly too mundane. Research should also seek to study these substances in the shamanic context, in which they have most effectively been used, designing appropriate test protocols for traditional settings.
Following in the footsteps of William James, Pablos returned to self-experimentation,136 developing a viable protocol for self-testing precognitive dreaming abilities with drugs, that might be adapted to waking experimentation as well. Experimental research should also be designed and conducted with an appreciative consideration of Tart’s proposals for the creation of state-specific sciences.137 Finally, as ever greater numbers of substances are discovered, and with a correspondingly large natural participant pool of psychedelic users, there is a need for more thorough and focused phenomenological research, that investigates and identifies the various types of paranormal experience that may occur in relation to each.138
David Luke
Literature
Abraham, H.D. (1983). Visual phenomenology of the LSD flashback. Archives of General Psychiatry 40, 884-9.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed). Arlington, Virginia, USA: American Psychiatric Association.
Asperen de Boer, S.R. van., Barkema, P.R., & Kappers, J. (1966). Is it possible to induce ESP with psilocybin? An exploratory investigation. International Journal of Neuropsychiatry 2, 447-73.
Black, D. (2001). Acid: A New Secret History of LSD. London: Vision Paperbacks.
Blewett, D. (1961). LSD in psychiatric treatment. In Proceedings of Two Conferences on Parapsychology and Pharmacology, by Anon., 56-58. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Blewett, D. (1963). Psychedelic drugs in parapsychological research. International Journal of Parapsychology 5, 43-74.
Brown, D.J. (2012). LSD & ESP: Scientists study psychic phenomena and psychedelic drugs. Santa Cruz Patch (online, 9 June, link defunct).
Cardeña, E. & Pekala, R. (2014). Researching states of consciousness and anomalous experiences. In Varieties of anomalous experience (2nd ed.) ed. by E. Cardeña, S.J. Lynn & S. Krippner, 21-56. Washington, DC:, USA: American Psychological Association.
Cardeña, E. (2009). Beyond Plato? Toward a science of alterations of consciousness. In Utrecht II: Charting the Future of Parapsychology, ed. by C.A. Roe, W. Kramer, & L. Coly, 305-22. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Cavanna, R., & Servadio, E. (1964). ESP experiments with LSD 25 and psilocybin. A Methodological Approach: Parapsychological Monographs 5. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Devereux, P. (1997). The Long Trip: A Prehistory of Psychedelia. New York: Penguin/Arkana.
Díaz, J.L. (1979). Ethnopharmacology and taxonomy of Mexican psychodysleptic plants. Journal of Psychedelic Drugs 11/1-2, 71-101.
Friedman, H. (1983). The Self-Expansiveness Level Form: A conceptualization and measurement of a transpersonal construct. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology 15, 37-50.
Garrett, E.J. (1961). Psychopharmacological parallels to mediumship. Proceedings on Two Conferences on Parapsychology and Pharmacology, 61-3. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Giesler, P. (1984). Parapsychological anthropology: I. Multi-method approaches to the study of psi in the field setting. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research 78, 287-328.
Grinspoon, L., & Bakalar, J.B. (1998). Psychedelic Drugs Reconsidered (2nd ed.). New York: Lindesmith Center.
Grob, C., & Harman. W. (1995). Making sense of the psychedelic issue: Federal approval for research into psychedelics resumes. Noetic Sciences Review 35, 4-19.
Grof, S. (1975). Realms of the Human Unconscious: Observations from LSD Research. New York: Viking Press.
Grof, S. (2001). LSD Psychotherapy (3rd ed.). Sarasota, Florida, USA: Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.
Hastings, A. (2006). An extended nondrug MDMA-like experience evoked through posthypnotic suggestion. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 38, 273-83.
Hermans, H.G.M. (1998). Memories of A Maverick. Maassluis, Netherlands: Pi.
Hirst, M. (2000). Root, dream and myth. The use of the oneirogenic plant Silene capensis among the Xhosa of South Africa. Eleusis: Journal of Psychoactive Plants and Compounds 4, 121-49.
Huxley, F. (1961). States of suggestibility. In Proceedings of Two Conferences on Parapsychology and Pharmacology, by Anon., 19-21. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Krippner, S. (1967). The cycle of deaths among U.S. presidents elected at twenty-year intervals. International Journal of Parapsychology 9/3, 145-53.
Krippner, S. (1999). A pilot study in dream telepathy with the Grateful Dead. In Perspectives on the Grateful Dead: Critical Writings ed. by R.G. Weiner, 11-17. Westport, Connecticut, USA: Greenwood Press.
Leary, T., Litwin. G. H., & Metzner, R. (1963). Reactions to psilocybin administered in a supportive environment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 137, 561-73.
Levine, J. (1968). Psychopharmacology: Implications for psi research. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 88-106. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Lilly, J.C. (1969). Parapsychological Association convention dinner address: Inner space and para-psychology [abstract]. Journal of Parapsychology 33, 349.
Ludwig, A.M. (1968). The hypnodelic state. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 68-87. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Ludwig, A.M., & Lyle, W.H. (1964). The experimental production of narcotic drug effects and withdrawal symptoms through hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 12, 1-17.
Luke, D. (2010). Anthropology and parapsychology: Still hostile sisters in science? Time and Mind: The Journal of Archaeology, Consciousness & Culture 3, 245-66.
Luke, D. (2011a). Anomalous phenomena, psi and altered consciousness. In Altering Consciousness: A Multidisciplinary Perspective, Volume 2: Biological and Psychological Perspectives, ed. by E. Cardeña & M. Winkelman, 355-74. Westport, Connecticut, USA: Praeger.
Luke, D. (2011b). Experiential reclamation and first person parapsychology. Journal of Parapsychology 75, 185-199.
Luke, D. (2014). Psychedelic possession: The growing incorporation of incorporation into ayahuasca use. In Talking with the Spirits: Ethnographies from Between the Worlds, ed by J. Hunter & D. Luke, 229-54. Brisbane, Australia: Daily Grail Publishing.
Luke, D.P. (2012). Psychoactive substances and paranormal phenomena: A comprehensive review. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies 31, 97-156.
Luke, D.P., & Kittenis, M. (2005). A preliminary survey of paranormal experiences with psychoactive drugs. Journal of Parapsychology 69/2, 305-27.
Masters, R.E.L., & Houston, J. (1966). The Varieties of Psychedelic Experience. London: Turnstone.
McKenna, D. (2004). Clinical investigations of the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: Rationale and regulatory challenges. Pharmacology and Therapeutics 102, 111-29.
Metzner, R. (1992). Divinatory dreams induced by tree datura. In Yearbook for Ethnomedicine and the Study of Consciousness,1991-0, ed. by C. Rätsch, 193-98. Berlin: VWB.
Metzner, R. (2005). Psychedelic, psychoactive and addictive drugs and states of consciousness. In Mind-Altering Drugs: The Science of Subjective Experience, ed. by M. Earlywine, 27-48. New York: Oxford University Press.
Millay, J. (2001). The influence of psychedelics on remote viewing. Bulletin of the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies 11/1, 43-4.
Mogar, R.E. (1965). Current status and future trends in psychedelic (LSD) research. Journal of Humanistic Psychology 5, 147-66.
Osis, K. (1961). A pharmacological approach to parapsychological experimentation. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 74-75. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Pablos, F. de. (2002). Enhancement of precognitive dreaming by cholinesterase inhibition: A pilot study. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research 66, 88-105.
Pahnke, W.N. (1968). The psychedelic mystical experience in terminal cancer patients and its possible implications for psi research. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 115-18. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Pahnke, W.N. (1971). The use of psychedelic drugs in parapsychological research. Parapsychology Review 2/4, 5-6 & 12-14.
Palmer, J. (1978). Extrasensory perception: Research findings. In Advances in Parapsychological Research: 2 Extrasensory Perception ed. by S. Krippner, 59-243). New York: Plenum Press.
Palmer, J. (1979). A community mail survey of psychic experiences. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research 73, 221-51.
Parker, A. (1975). States of Mind: ESP and Altered States of Consciousness. London: Malaby Press.
Pitman, J.A., & Owens, N.E. (2004). The effect of manipulating expectations before and during a test of ESP. Journal of Parapsychology, 68/1, 45-63.
Puharich, A. (1962). Beyond Telepathy. Garden City, New York: Doubleday & Co.
Rogo, D.S. (1976). Exploring Psychic Phenomena: Beyond Mind and Matter. Wheaton, Illinois, USA: The Theosophical Society in America.
Ryzl, M. (1968). Training methods for psi induction. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 55-67). New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Shanon, B. (2003). Hallucinations. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10/2, 3-31.
Smythies, J.R. (1960). New research frontiers in parapsychology and pharmacology. International Journal of Parapsychology, 2/2, 28-38.
Sobiecki, J. F. (2012). Psychoactive ubulawu spiritual medicines and healing dynamics in the initiation process of southern Bantu diviners. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 44, 216-23.
Stevens, J. (1988). Storming Heaven: LSD and the American Dream. London: William Heinemann.
Stokes, D.M. (1997). Spontaneous psi phenomena. In Advances in Parapsychological Research 8, ed. by S. Krippner, 6-87. Jefferson, North Carolina, USA: McFarland.
Storm, L., & Rock, A.J. (2011). Shamanism and Psi: Imagery Cultivation as an Alternative to the Ganzfeld Protocol. Gladesville, New South Wales, Australia: Australian Institute of Parapsychological Research.
Strassman, R. [J.] (2001). DMT: The Spirit Molecule: A Doctor’s Revolutionary Research into the Biology of Near-Death and Mystical Experiences. Rochester, Vermont, USA: Park Street Press.
Strassman, R.J., Qualls, C.R., Uhlenhuth, E.H., & Kellner, R. (1994). Dose-response study of N,N-dimethyltryptamine in humans: II Subjective effects and preliminary results of a new rating scale. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 98-108.
Tart, C.T. (1968). Hypnosis, psychedelics, and psi: Conceptual models. In Psi and Altered States of Consciousness: Proceedings of an International Conference on Hypnosis, Drugs, Dreams, and Psi, ed. by R. Cavanna & M. Ullman, 24-41. New York: Parapsychology Foundation.
Tart, C.T. (1972). States of consciousness and state-specific sciences. Science 176, 1203-10.
Tart, C.T. (1977). Drug-induced states of consciousness. In Handbook of Parapsychology, ed. by B. Wolman, 500-525). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Tart, C.T. (1993). Marijuana intoxication, psi, and spiritual experiences. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research 87, 149-70.
Tinoco, C.A. (1994). Testa de ESP em pacientes sob efeito da ayahuasca [Controlled ESP test in patients under the influence of ayahuasca]. Revista Brasileira de Parapsicologia 14, 42-8.
Vayne, J. (2001). Pharmakon: Drugs and the Imagination. London: Liminalspace/El Cheapo.
Wezelman, R., & Bierman, D. J. (1997). Process orientated ganzfeld research in Amsterdam. Proceedings of the 40th Parapsychology Association Annual Convention held in conjunction with the Society for Psychical Research 477-92.
Winkelman, M. (1983). The anthropology of magic and parapsychological research. Parapsychological Review 14/2, 13-19.
Wulff, D.M. (2014). Mystical experiences. In Varieties of Anomalous Experience: Examining the Scientific Evidence (2nd ed.), ed. by E. Cardeña, S.J. Lynn, & S. Krippner, 369-408). Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association.
Endnotes
- 1. Grinspoon & Bakalar (1998).
- 2. Devereux (1997).
- 3. Luke (2010).
- 4. Winkelman (1983).
- 5. Smythies (1960).
- 6. Luke (2012).
- 7. Black (2001).
- 8. Luke (2012).
- 9. Luke (2012).
- 10. Krippner (1967).
- 11. Luke (2012).
- 12. Luke (2012).
- 13. Mogar (1965).
- 14. Abraham (1983).
- 15. American Psychiatric Association (2013).
- 16. Luke (2012).
- 17. Grof (1975, 2001).
- 18. Grof (2001).
- 19. Cardeña & Pekala (2014); Stokes (1997).
- 20. Shanon (2003).
- 21. Shanon (2003).
- 22. Luke (2012).
- 23. Tart (1993).
- 24. Stokes (1997).
- 25. Palmer (1979).
- 26. Luke (2012).
- 27. Metzner (2005).
- 28. Luke (2012).
- 29. Luke & Kittenis (2005).
- 30. Cardeña (2009); Luke, D. (2011a).
- 31. Palmer (1978).
- 32. Luke (2012).
- 33. Luke (2012).
- 34. Palmer (1978).
- 35. Asperen de Boer, Barkema & Kappers (1966).
- 36. Parker (1975).
- 37. Asperen de Boer, Barkema & Kappers (1966).
- 38. Luke (2012).
- 39. Puharich (1962).
- 40. Luke (2012).
- 41. Asperen de Boer, Barkema, & Kappers (1966).
- 42. Millay (2001); Rogo, D.S. (1976).
- 43. Luke (2012).
- 44. Parker (1975).
- 45. Luke (2012).
- 46. Osis (1961); Smythies (1960).
- 47. Osis (1961)
- 48. Blewett (1963).
- 49. Millay (2001); Parker (1975).
- 50. Ryzl (1968).
- 51. Lilly (1969).
- 52. Ryzl (1968).
- 53. Blewett (1961, 1963).
- 54. Huxley (1961).
- 55. Blewett (1963); Parker (1975); Tart (1977).
- 56. Wulff (2014).
- 57. Bierman (1998, October); Masters & Houston (1966); Wezelman & Bierman (1997).
- 58. Luke (2012).
- 59. Millay (2001); Tart (1977).
- 60. Levine (1968).
- 61. Blewett (1963); Millay (2001); Parker (1975).
- 62. Pahnke (1968).
- 63. Tart (1968).
- 64. Parker (1975); Tart (1968).
- 65. Ludwig (1968).
- 66. Ryzl (1968).
- 67. Ludwig (1968).
- 68. Hastings (2006); Ludwig & Lyle (1964).
- 69. Rogo (1976).
- 70. Smythies (1960); Levine (1968).
- 71. Garrett (1961).
- 72. Osis (1961).
- 73. Luke (2014).
- 74. Ryzl (1968).
- 75. Grof (2001).
- 76. Masters & Houston (1966).
- 77. Pahnke (1971).
- 78. Asperen de Boer, Barkema, & Kappers (1966); Tinoco (1994).
- 79. Osis (1961).
- 80. Pahnke (1971).
- 81. Asperen de Boer, Barkema, & Kappers (1966).
- 82. Cavanna & Servadio (1964).
- 83. Blewett (1963).
- 84. Ryzl (1968).
- 85. Luke (2012).
- 86. Tart (1993).
- 87. Puharich (1962).
- 88. Pablos (2002).
- 89. Devereux (1997); Díaz (1979).
- 90. Hirst (2000); Sobiecki (2012).
- 91. Metzner (1992).
- 92. Brown (2012).
- 93. Luke & Kittenis (2005).
- 94. Luke & Kittenis (2005).
- 95. Cavanna & Servadio (1964).
- 96. Pitman & Owens (2004).
- 97. Levine (1968).
- 98. Blewett (1961).
- 99. Strassman (2001).
- 100. Tart (1977).
- 101. Wezelman & Bierman (1997).
- 102. Tart (1977).
- 103. Strassman, Qualls, Uhlenhuth & Kellner (1994).
- 104. Friedman (1983).
- 105. Parker (1975); Tart (1968). 41).
- 106. Leary, Litwin, & Metzner (1963).
- 107. Vayne (2001).
- 108. Levine (1968); Tart (1968).
- 109. Blewett (1963).
- 110. Parker (1975); Tart (1968).
- 111. Blewett (1963).
- 112. Blewett (1963); Parker (1975).
- 113. Cavanna & Servadio (1964).
- 114. Strassman (2001).
- 115. Tart (1968).
- 116. Levine (1968).
- 117. Tart (1977).
- 118. Grof (2001); Tart (1968).
- 119. Grob & Harman (1995).
- 120. Giesler (1984); Luke (2010).
- 121. Storm & Rock (2011).
- 122. Tart (1977).
- 123. Luke (2011b).
- 124. Cardeña & Pekala (2014).
- 125. Grob & Harman (1995).
- 126. McKenna (2004); Strassman (2001).
- 127. Tart (1977).
- 128. Krippner (1999).
- 129. Giesler (1984).
- 130. Stevens (1988).
- 131. Luke & Kittenis (2005).
- 132. Hermans (1998).
- 133. Grob & Harman (1995).
- 134. Luke (2012).
- 135. Bierman (1998, October).
- 136. Pablos (2002).
- 137. Tart (1972).
- 138. Luke & Kittenis (2005).